We humans are perhaps unique among all species of animal for the complexity of the process by which we select a mate.
A male riflebird dances for a female. A good dance equals good genes, which is top of the female's very short list of "must haves" in a partner. Source: Francesco Veronesi /Flickr |
True, male puffer fish strive to impress females by constructing
elaborate flower-shaped nests on the sandy sea floor; male Victoria’s
riflebirds perform
rhythmic dances to best show off their glittering
blue feathers; and female túngara frogs, who value bulk in a mate, seek
out males with the deepest, most sonorous croak. But while these
behaviors may be complex, the systems are relatively simple. Females
judge males on their nest building ability, their dancing, or their
croaks, but don’t tend to have a long shopping list of must-haves in a
partner.
Humans, however, vary in our preferences and are prone to trade off one desirable trait
against another. You might want a physically attractive partner, but
also someone who is honest, dependable, ambitious, kind, generous, and
the list goes on. No one (except Ryan Gosling) can hope to score a
perfect 10 in every category. This is why most of us must decide whether
the constellation of traits possessed by each prospective partner meets
the grade. Perhaps John is high on sex appeal but low on earning potential, while Sam is rich but looks like a foot?
Some might find that a tough choice, and many research studies have
sought to investigate how, and why, humans trade one trait off against
another.
Further complicating the matter is a question recently posed by Gul
Gunaydin of Bilkent University in Ankara, Turkey. She and her colleagues
realized that in all of the existing research studies, volunteers had
been tasked with choosing between two alternative partners who were new.
“But,” she asked, “what if one of the alternatives is the current
partner?”
Of course, this is likely to be a question that many of us who have a
long-term partner grapple with often. Just because a person has a
partner, doesn’t mean that they close their eyes to all alternatives.
Choosing a partner isn’t a one-off decision, but a continuous process
characterized by repeated decisions to either stick with the
relationship or to jump ship for someone new.
Stick or twist?
In a series of experiments, Gunaydin had her research volunteers
imagine they were in this situation: they had been in a relationship for
three months, but recently were introduced to someone new. This person
was compared with the current partner, and described either as more
trustworthy but less attractive, or less trustworthy but more attractive. Would the volunteer consider ditching their current partner for the new alternative: yes or no?
Other volunteers had to choose between partners differing in
attractiveness and wealth, or wealth and trustworthiness. A control
group of volunteers were asked to imagine that they were single, but
otherwise the task was the same: to choose between two potential
partners.
The results of the experiments showed that “partnered” volunteers
erred toward sticking with their current partner. That is, if the
partner was attractive but untrustworthy, the volunteer overvalued
attractiveness; if the partner was trustworthy but unattractive, the
volunteer overvalued trustworthiness.
So, our preferences for the status quo seem to be stronger than our
preferences for any particular trait. We can trade one desired trait off
against another, but are also carried along by inertia. It’s easier to
stick with your current partner, even if he or she is imaginary!
Festival of deception
But, what if this effect only emerges when the situation is
hypothetical? To test whether status quo preferences persist under more
naturalistic conditions, Gunaydin set up another experiment. This one
was more fun because it involved deceiving the volunteers, which we
shouldn’t begrudge psychologists because it’s pretty much the only thing
they enjoy doing.
Gunaydin invited each female volunteer to the lab one at a time. Each
woman was told she would be taking part in an experiment about decision-making
in interpersonal relationships. They would make a series of decisions
in concert with another volunteer — a man — whom they would meet
momentarily. Half of the female volunteers would be randomly allocated a
male partner, but the other half would read profiles of different men
and get to choose which one they preferred to interact with.
In reality, the experiment wasn’t about “decision-making in interpersonal relationships” and all of
the women got to choose which man they preferred to talk with. The
profiles were also fake, and had been written by the researchers. Each
woman was given three profiles printed onto paper. Two of them described
awful men, who no one in their right mind would choose, but the third
man had good points as well as bad and was almost always chosen. After
the volunteer had chosen her preferred man, she handed the papers back
to the research assistant. The research assistant then apologized: “Oh
no, did I give you three profiles earlier? There should have
been one more.” They would then go fetch a fourth profile for the
volunteer to consider. And, you guessed it, the man described in this
new profile was the opposite of the previously chosen man. If the
volunteer had chosen to speak with a wealthy but untrustworthy man, the
new man was poor but trustworthy, and vice versa.
Would the volunteers stick with the man they had chosen first, or would they switch to the new man?
The results of the experiment showed that people were more likely to
stick with the man they had chosen first, even though in this instance
the commitment had lasted little more than a minute or two.
Gunaydin and her colleagues wonder whether people would:
“…still prefer the status quo if they encountered potential partners in
the flesh? Future research is needed to address this question.”
They also conclude that their results suggest that mate-choice can’t
be considered an entirely rational process, since a rational process
would involve weighing the better or most preferred trait more highly,
regardless of whether that trait is possessed by a new or established
partner. However, they also acknowledge that, across multiple domains,
humans hate to lose out more than we love to gain: so called “loss aversion”. Mate-choice may not be special: we prefer the status quo generally.
One upside of this preference may be that it keeps our relationships
intact. If we changed our affections every time we met someone who
surpassed our partners on one trait, many of our relationships would be
very brief indeed.
Unless, of course, we manage to shack up with Ryan Gosling. Back off: I saw him first.
Robert Burriss, Ph.D., is an evolutionary psychologist at Basel University in Switzerland. He produces The Psychology of Attractiveness Podcast.
No comments:
Post a Comment