All day long, while we’re awake, our brains are busy. They take in
the world, learn from our experiences, and form memories of what we have
seen, done, heard and learned. Sleep plays an essential role in
consolidating learning and memory. How exactly that works, however, is
still mostly a mystery. This week, at the Society for Neuroscience’s annual meeting, researchers presented a few new clues as to what might be happening in the brain while we are asleep.
Scientists have previously been able to watch the neural patterns—the
sequence in which brain cells fire—that reactivate waking experiences
as animals sleep. In other words, they’ve pinpointed memories in
animals’ brains. Memories aren’t stored in a particular place in the
brain. Instead, a memory represents a particular sequence of brain
activity.
But in humans, that’s been harder to observe. That’s why these new
results, still unpublished, are so intriguing. They showed, in
humans, that sleep selectively boosts negative memories. And by
analyzing electrical brain patterns during sleep with machine learning
algorithms, scientists successfully predicted what kind of content
people had learned right before they went to bed.
“Curiously sleep does not treat all memories equally,” says Roy Cox, a cognitive
neuroscientist at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard
Medical School, who investigated negative memories. More than 50 healthy
people came into Cox’s lab. First, participants learned a memory task
in which they were shown photographs with either negative or neutral
content, such as a burning house versus a house cat. They were tested on
their memories of both types of content. Then for the next 12 hours,
half the group slept (they came in at night), and half stayed awake
(they came in during the day). When they were retested after 12 hours,
the group that had been awake forgot negative and neutral information at
equal rates. But the group that slept was different. They forgot the
neutral information at the same rate as the other group, but they
remembered the negative images much better. “A night of sleep seems to
prevent forgetting of negative items specifically,” Cox says.
He and his colleagues believe that it might be beneficial for the
brain to pick out negative memories—such as the appearance and location
of a tasty-looking berry that turned out to make you sick—to protect
against those threats in the future.
The scientists were also able to trick the brain into storing the
negative and neutral memories differently (they took advantage of the
fact that information seen in the left visual field is processed in the
right visual cortex and vice versa). So all the negative memories were
processed on one side of the brain and the neutral memories on the other
side. They plan to use the resulting distinct neural signatures,
recorded with electroencephalography (EEG), to delve further into
whether the negative pattern of brain activity is reactivated more
strongly during sleep.
In another study, Monika Schönauer,
a neuroscientist at the University of Töbingen in Germany, was able to
visualize memory processing during sleep in humans. Like Cox, she was
using EEG, which traces the firing activity of neurons over time. But
that technique generates reams of data. “We observe the silent activity
of 10,000 neurons,” says Schönauer. The specific activity that relates
to previous learning represents a tiny fraction of that EEG readout. So
Schönauer and her colleagues tested whether they could use a machine
learning algorithm—essentially throwing the power of computers at the
problem—to recognize differences in brain activity based on differences
in what an individual had previously learned.
o do that, they had participants came into the lab for a night. Before
going to bed, half the group was shown images of faces, the other half
of houses—lots and lots of images. Then they slept for eight hours. “We
recorded electrical brain activity during this time,” explained
Schönauer. Then they tested whether their algorithm could predict
whether a participant had learned about faces or houses before sleep. It
could, accurately predicting the content 65 percent of the time.
No comments:
Post a Comment