Wouldn’t it be great if you could automatically read the emotions of
the important people in your life? How nice to know, without their
telling you, whether they’re sad or happy, scared or calm, and angry or
pleased. You could adjust your own reactions to their feeling state
without having to exchange a word, and you would almost never have to
deal with the misunderstandings that occur when people get confused
about each other’s feelings. It is true that the better you know
someone, the better able you are to decipher their facial cues, but this
is not always a guarantee, even if you’ve lived with your partner for
years.
The ability to detect emotions remains one of the most fascinating
areas of research on interpersonal relations. Because there is no
completely direct pathway from emotions to facial cues and then to the
interpretation of those cues, researchers must devise clever ways to
trace these complex connections. New technologies in the lab are making
it possible to present endless variations on the basic human expressions
with stimuli designed to overcome the traditional limitations in which a
flesh-and-blood face’s attractiveness
is
confounded with that person’s facial expressions. People can be
created via computer graphics as abstract images so that the
experimenter manipulates only the most relevant features that contribute
to the way that participants interpret that face’s emotions.
Anderson University’s Robert Franklin, Jr., and colleagues (2019),
noting that “the face gives people their best glimpse into the otherwise
invisible mental and emotional processes that occur in others’ minds”
(p. 209). Moreover, getting this glimpse is important because “emotions
convey functional information about others’ probable behavior toward a
perceiver,” or “behavioral forecasts.” In other words, if you want to
know what the person you’re dealing with is going to do, you’ll get the
most information possible from that person’s facial expressions. The
two cues that seem most germane to predicting people’s behavior,
Franklin et al. argue, are what they call angularity and roundness.
Angularity is a predictor of anger, they maintain, and roundness suggests joy.
Think now about what your face does when you are angry. You feel your
eyebrows point downward into a frown, and you may even turn the edges
of your mouth downward too. Conversely, when you’re feeling happy or
pleased, the lines in your face soften as you allow everything to relax.
Your eyes may crinkle when you smile, but your eyebrows look less like
arrows and more like commas. Moving beyond just what your face does,
however, Franklin and his colleagues cite previous research supporting
the idea that negative emotions in general are associated with lines and
jagged edges, but that positive emotions are associated with curves and
circles. Angularity is associated with threat, they point out, and
roundness with safety.
Moving on from the association between threat and angularity and
safety with roundness, the downward facing V of the eyebrows of the
angry person plus the X-shaped angry mouth would signal that you’re
under threat. You’d regard the softer curves of the happy person as
suggesting that nothing bad will happen to you.
To test the idea that facial angularity signals anger and roundness
signals joy, Franklin and his fellow researchers designed
computer-generated stimuli of faces that blurred everything except the
abstract angularity or roundness cues. An “X” shaped face, then, has
downward facing eyebrows and downward-turning edges of the mouth. A
diamond-shaped face communicates has as eyebrows the inverted “V” and as
a mouth a smile that roughly fits the “V” pattern. The preliminary
pilot findings showed that, indeed, by superimposing these angles onto
actual human faces depicting differing emotions, the faces intended to
show anger fit indeed matched the “X” pattern and those intended to show
joy fit the diamond shape.
With this connection between angularity/roundness and anger/joy
established, the research team then presented a sample of 33
undergraduates with a set of 8 faces (4 male and 4 female) showing
either angry or joyful expressions. In one set of faces, the outward
lines were filtered out, and in the other, the inward lines were
filtered. The test of the hypothesis involved asking participants to
rate the expressions shown in these faces, with the expectation being
that participants would perform emotion judgments more quickly when the
lines that were filtered matched the emotion depicted in the face.
People took longer to judge the emotion of anger when some lines showing
roundness were left in the filtered picture, and longer to judge joy
when they could still see the X-shaped lines in the faces they were
judging.
The final investigation tested how angularity and roundness would
affect the judgments made of neutral faces. Using, once again, an
undergraduate sample, the researchers were able to establish that even
when the face in the picture had no overt expression, filtering the
images to accentuate angularity or roundness led participants to
interpret the emotions as anger or joy, respectively.
From a theoretical perspective, the authors note that there could be
adaptive reasons that people associate angularity with anger and
roundness with joy. Returning to the idea that angularity poses a
threat, Franklin and his collaborators suggest that these cues may be
learned early in development. Angular- i.e. sharp- things hurt, but
round things (for the most part) do not. You could drop a stone on your
toe and that would hurt, but you’re less likely to tear your skin with a
stone than with a twig. Another possibility is that because round
faces look younger and more babyish than angular faces, which reflect
greater age and maturity, people feel less threatened by them. You are
also more likely to regard as “cute” the soft round features of a baby
or young child. That image of “Hello Kitty” is one that people are
drawn to for similar reasons.
To sum up, the findings have two implications.
First, when you’re looking at other people’s faces, those V’s and X’s
signal whether you’ve made them angry or happy. Second, look at your own
facial expressions. Are you inadvertently looking angry when you really
feel pleasure? Finding fulfillment in relationships
with others relies heavily on the expression and reading of emotions.
The Franklin et al. study shows how those emotions play out on your
chief signaling device—your face.
About the Author
Susan Krauss Whitbourne, Ph.D., is a Professor Emerita of Psychological and Brain Sciences at the University of Massachusetts Amherst. Her latest book is The Search for Fulfillment.
No comments:
Post a Comment