Sarah Shourd’s mind began to slip after about two months into her
incarceration. She heard phantom footsteps and flashing lights, and
spent most of her day crouched on all fours, listening through a gap in
the door.
“In the periphery of my vision, I
began to see flashing lights, only to jerk my head around to find that
nothing was there,” she wrote in the New York Times in 2011.
“At one point, I heard someone screaming, and it wasn’t until I felt
the hands of one of the friendlier guards on my face, trying to revive
me, that I realised the screams were my own.”
We all want to be alone from time to time, to escape the demands of our colleagues or the hassle of crowds. But not alone
alone. For most people, prolonged social isolation is all bad,
particularly mentally. We know this not only from reports by people like
Shourd who have experienced it first-hand, but also from psychological
experiments on the effects of isolation and sensory deprivation, some of
which had to be called off due to the extreme and bizarre reactions of
those involved. Why does the mind unravel so spectacularly when we’re
truly on our own, and is there any way to stop it?
We’ve known for a while that isolation is physically bad for us. Chronically lonely people have higher blood pressure, are more vulnerable to infection, and are also more likely to develop Alzheimer’s disease and dementia.
Loneliness also interferes with a whole range of everyday functioning,
such as sleep patterns, attention and logical and verbal reasoning. The
mechanisms behind these effects are still unclear, though what is known
is that social isolation unleashes an extreme immune response – a
cascade of stress hormones and inflammation. This may have been
appropriate in our early ancestors, when being isolated from the group
carried big physical risks, but for us the outcome is mostly harmful.
Yet
some of the most profound effects of loneliness are on the mind. For
starters, isolation messes with our sense of time. One of the strangest
effects is the ‘time-shifting’ reported by those who have spent long periods living underground without daylight.
In 1961, French geologist Michel Siffre led a two-week expedition to
study an underground glacier beneath the French Alps and ended up
staying two months, fascinated by how the darkness affected human
biology. He decided to abandon his watch and “live like an animal”.
While conducting tests with his team on the surface, they discovered it
took him five minutes to count to what he thought was 120 seconds.
No comments:
Post a Comment